Touch In Psychotherapy And The Standard Of Care

By Ofer Zur, Ph.D.
 

 

 

 
Abstract

The question of touch in therapy has been debated since the inception of the field early in the last century. The main concern about physical contact in therapy has focused on the sexually exploitative therapists and the concern that a client may interpret touch as having sexual intent. Ignoring years of clinical and developmental research, many risk management experts, traditional psychoanalysts, consumer protection agencies, insurance companies and malpractice attorneys have promoted the notion that any touch beyond a handshake is clinically inappropriate, unethical or below the standard of care. Drawing on the faulty slippery slope theory that even appropriate boundary crossings are likely to lead to boundary violations, they assert that even scientifically proven, appropriate and clinically helpful touch is likely to lead to unethical sexual touch. The aim of this paper is to clarify the relationship between professional therapeutic touch and the standard of care. To achieve this goal the paper defines the standard of care in psychotherapy, and details the elements of the standard and articulates what the standard is and is not. It then briefly reviews the clinical research on touch in therapy and identifies the different types of touch employed in therapy. The paper then articulates, in detail, how non-sexual, clinically appropriate and therapeutic touch falls within the standard of care of psychotherapy and counseling. Additionally, the paper discusses issues, as they relate to touch in therapy, of theoretical orientation, codes of ethics, risk management, differences between sexual and non-sexual touch, and it reviews the idea of the slippery slope. Finally, the paper outlines how therapists, who appropriately use touch in therapy, can demonstrate compliance with the standard of care.

Top of Page
 

Introduction: Forces And Influences In The Battle Of Touch

While touch has been part of most healing traditions throughout human history, it has been controversial in the western medicine and more so within the field of psychotherapy and counseling (Aposhyan, 2004; Smith, Clance, & Imes, 1998, Totton, 2005; Young, 2005). The main concern around the issue of therapeutic touch has been that psychotherapists and counselors may use their power and influence to sexually exploit their clients (Pope & Vasquez, 2007; Rutter, 1989). The second concern has been that a client may interpret touch as having sexual intent.A As a result, since the field’s inception the application of touch in psychotherapy has been one of the most hotly debated topics (Hunter & Struve, 1998; Zur, 2007). Whilst Freud initially endorsed the use of touch as part of psychotherapy, he changed his position entirely in the early 1920s when he became worried how the use of touch might tarnish the reputation of the new field. The issue erupted when Freud, back in 1931, scolded his star student and disciple, Ferenczi, for letting a female client kiss him (Young, 2005). Freud felt that physical contact would lead to sexual enactments, and, by his own admission, he was equally concerned with the reputation of psychoanalysis, thus forcing the issue of touch to go underground. Ferenczi refused to stop touching his clients altogether and was subsequently expelled from the ranks of orthodox psychoanalysis (Fosshage, 2000). Wilhelm Reich (1972), who developed the most comprehensive method of clinical touch, was, like Ferenczi, one of Freud’s inner circle and prominent in the prestigious International Psychoanalytic Association (IPA). He, too, was ousted from the International Psychoanalytic Association for his professional stance on touch in therapy. As psychoanalysis emerged, an analytic ideology was created around the prohibition of touch. It was based on the conviction that any touch is likely to gratify sexual and instinctual infantile longings or drives, subsequently contaminating the analytic container and nullifying the possibilities for analysis to help the clients work through their issues (Langs, 1982; Simon, 1994).

The conflict around the use of touch in therapy has stayed with the field since that time. In recent years the primary tension is between, on one side, the long-established scientific knowledge that has consistently proven that touch is essential for healthy human development and human relationships and, on the other side, the ethical concerns with exploitative and harmful sexual touching of clients by therapists. A great amount of scientific data has been acquired in the last half century on the importance of touch for human development, bonding, communication and healing by the classic work of Bowlby (1969), Harlow (1971) and Montagu (1986) and more recently, the extremely prestigious Field (2003). The clinical use of touch in therapy has also been studied extensively and has conclusively determined that touch can enhance the therapeutic alliance as well as increase a sense of trust, calm and safety (Smith, et al., 1998). Touch has also been shown to be effective in the treatment of depression, anxiety, PTSD and other mental disorders and conditions (e.g., Aposhyan, 2004; Field, 1998, 2003; Hunter & Struve, 1998; May, 2005; Young, 2005). On the other side there is a major concern, raised mainly by risk management experts, ethical review boards, insurance companies and consumer protection agencies, that nonsexual touch can lead to sexual touch and exploitation of clients (Bersoff, 1999; Gabbard, 1994; Pope & Vasquez, 2007; Rutter, 1989; Simon, 1991, 1994). There is little ‘evidence’ for this fear, though – as in any profession – an occasional therapist has been successfully charged with inappropriate touch.

Another rift in the field stems from different therapeutic philosophies. On one side there are the analytic practitioners, on another those who focus on biological-pharmacological intervention, and who advocate a hands-off approach for philosophical-clinical reasons. On another side of the debate are humanistic, group, family, cognitive-behavioral and feminist therapists who see value in appropriate touch and other boundary crossings, such as self-disclosure, gift exchange, bartering and dual relationships (Williams, 1997; Zur, 2007). These greatly outnumber the first group. Bodypsychotherapists, many of whom use touch as a primary clinical tool, obviously believe in the importance of touch in general and in its scientifically established clinical utility in particular (Aposhyan, 2004; Nordmarken & Zur, 2004; Young, 2005).

Several psychotherapists’ surveys over the years revealed that 87% of therapists touch their clients (Tirnauer, Smith, & Foster, 1996), 85% hug their clients (Pope, Tabachnick, & Keith-Spiegel, 1987) and 65% approve of touch as an adjunct to verbal psychotherapy (Schultz, 1975). In a more recent survey Stenzel & Rupert (2004) reported a decrease in the general reporting of the use of touch in therapy, which they, reasonably, partly attributed to the increase in the dominance of risk management training. They also reported a significant increase in reporting of female therapists touching female clients. The reported decrease may also be the result of a biased sample as more therapists are not only trained in risk management but also have been intimidated by it and, therefore, are less likely to admit to touching clients, by either refusing to participate or by declining to admit to touch practices.

Some of the negative and frightening messages regarding touch, that psychotherapists have been inundated with, come from prominent therapists, many of who are psychoanalytically oriented. One example is Menninger who asserts that physical contact with a patient is “evidence of incompetence or criminal ruthlessness of the analysts” (cited in Horton, Clance, Sterk-Elifson, & Emshoff, 1995, p. 444). Simon, in a similar vein, instructs therapists to “Foster psychological separateness of the patient. . . interact only verbally with clients. . . minimize physical contact” (1994, p. 514). Wolberg (1967) agrees: “Physical contact with the patient is absolutely a taboo (since it may) mobilize sexual feelings in the patient and the therapist, or bring forth violent outbursts of anger” (p. 606). These extremely small, but biased perspectives, have created an inappropriately weighted bias, both in the profession’s and in the public’s mind.

Similarly to the psychoanalytic attitude towards touch in therapy, strong messages were pronounced by risk management or defensive medicine experts who often place touch at the top of the ‘Do not do’ list. “From the viewpoint of current risk-management principles,” Gutheil & Gabbard stated, “a handshake is about the limit of social physical contact at this time” (1993, p. 195). Similarly, the popular Web site, WebMD (1992), announces “A Hug-Free Zone: The threat of lawsuits, the already strong language in the APA code, and the general litigiousness of society have prompted many therapists to erect barriers between themselves and their clients when it comes to any physical contact. No more hugs for a sobbing client. No encouraging pats on the back” (section 2, Para. 1). Risk management has been defined in realistic and pragmatic terms by Gutheil & Gabbard (1993) and Williams (1997, 2003) as the course by which therapists refrain from implementing certain interventions because they may be misinterpreted and questioned by boards, ethics committees and courts. Obviously, these practices almost exclusively serve to protect the practitioners, not the consumers. At the core of the risk management injunctions against touch in therapy is the belief in the ‘slippery slope’. This is the idea that failure to adhere to hands-off, rigid standards will most likely lead to therapist-client sexual relationships.

Historically, the 1960s and 1970s witnessed a general increase in litigious attitudes in the culture at large and in the rise of defensive medicine and risk management practices in the field of medicine, including psychotherapy and counseling. Following the sexual revolution of the 1960s and sexual digressions by some sections of Gestalt Therapy and Humanistic Psychology at Esalen in CA, risk-management teachings have strongly encouraged psychotherapists to avoid almost all forms of touch and most other boundary crossings or deviation from analytic hands-off practices. The concern during this time, as is also reflected in the professional associations’ codes of ethics, was that any deviation from analytic-type practices are likely to lead to sexual and other violations of clients by permissible therapists. A ‘bogey-man’ attitude had therefore been created, based almost totally on bias and fears of litigation.

At the end of the 20th century and the beginning of the new millennium, there have been two significant and contradictory forces that have affected the relationships between boundaries in general, including touch and the attitudes towards therapeutic boundaries, and the perception of the standard of care. On the one hand, risk management, in regard to touch issues and other boundary considerations, has continued to be a concern for professional organizations and consumer protection agencies. On the other hand, there has been a significant increase in the number of publications that associate boundary crossings with increased therapeutic effectiveness (e.g., Younggren & Gottlieb, 2004; Zur, 2007). As articulated below, a similar shift has taken place regarding touch in therapy as illustrated by the 1998 American Psychologist publication of Field’s article, “Massage therapy effects,” and several other publications (e.g., Hunter & Struve, 1998; Smith, et al., 1998; White, 2002; Young, 2005) along with the establishment of United States Association of Body Psychotherapists Journal and the European Association of Body Psychotherapists, which both advocate that properly trained, body-oriented psychotherapists are not only the only people ‘qualified’ to touch, but that a body-oriented approach, which might involve touch, is considerably as effective, and some (i.e,. Young, 2005) say even more effective than a psychotherapy that totally ignores or that does not relate to the patient’s body in any way.

Top of Page
 

Therapeutic Boundaries And Touch In Therapy

Boundaries in therapy, including the boundary issues that involve touch, are extremely important. They define the therapeutic fiduciary relationships and distinguish psychotherapy from social, sexual, business and many other types of relationships, also having a direct impact on the effectiveness of therapy. There are two types of boundaries. One type is where boundaries are drawn around the therapeutic relationship and involve issues of fees, privacy and confidentiality, and place and time of therapy. Boundaries of another sort are drawn between therapist and client, rather than around them. Touch between therapist and client is an obvious boundary of this latter kind as well as therapists’ self-disclosure and giving and receiving gifts (Gutheil & Gabbard, 1993). Touch between therapist and client represents one of the most recognized psychotherapeutic boundaries, as it reaches across the professional-interpersonal space separating therapist and client (Zur, 2007).

Boundaries in therapy have been regarded as the “edge” of appropriate behavior (Gutheil & Gabbard, 1993) and involve two types of boundaries: boundary crossings and boundary violations. A boundary violation occurs when a therapist crosses the line of decency and integrity and misuses his/her power to exploit a client for the therapist’s own benefit. Therapist-client sexual relationships are a prime example of boundary violations. Such violations may also involve any exploitive business or other relationships and are always counter-clinical, unethical and are often illegal. In contrast, boundary crossings involve courtesy or ritualistic gestures, such as a handshake or a pat on the back. They have been defined as any deviation from traditional psychoanalytic practices (Zur, 2007). Boundary crossings also involve clinically effective interventions and are part of a well-constructed treatment plan, such as clinically and ethically appropriate self-disclosure, home visits, gift exchange or bartering (Herlihy & Corey, 2006; Lazarus & Zur, 2002). As will be articulated in this paper, while boundary violations are inherently unethical and always below the standard of care, boundary crossings are neither unethical, nor below the standard of care.

Boundary crossings are often an integrated part of most practiced therapeutic modalities, such as humanistic, somatic, cognitive behavioral or group therapy. Following are just a few examples of beneficial boundary crossings and the corresponding theoretical orientations that are likely to support them. Behavioral therapy readily endorses flying on an airplane with a fear-of-flying client as part of an exposure or in-vivo intervention. Cognitive, behavioral and cognitive-behavioral therapies endorse self-disclosure as a way of modeling, offering an alternative perspective or exemplifying cognitive flexibility. Humanistic, feminist and existential therapies endorse self-disclosure as a way of enhancing authentic connections and increasing therapeutic alliance, the best predictor of therapeutic success. A client-initiated handshake at the beginning or end of a session, an appropriate and encouraging pat on the client’s back, supportive handholding or a nonsexual hug can be exceedingly clinically helpful. They are all considered boundary crossings and are endorsed by most therapeutic modalities. Not to put a consoling arm around a client who has suddenly burst into tears, might be seen, not only as uncaring, but in fact a rigid application of therapeutic boundaries. Additionally, specially trained body psychotherapists, such as Reichian or bioenergetic therapists, who use thoroughly researched and established hands-on techniques, are also engaged in therapeutic boundary crossings.

Dual relationships are a type of boundary consideration that often has been misunderstood and mischaracterized. Dual relationships take place when therapists and clients engage in additional social, business or professional relationships other than the traditional one-on-one therapist-client relationship (Lazarus & Zur, 2002). Sexual relationships between therapists and current clients are obviously totally inappropriate dual relationships and are also boundary violations, always counter-clinical, unethical and illegal in most states (Pope & Vasquez, 2007). Non-sexual and non-exploitative social and other dual relationships are often unavoidable in rural communities, university and college campuses and other small communities, and they can also be beneficial to therapy (Herlihy & Corey, 2006; Schank & Skovholt, 2006; Younggren & Gottlieb, 2004; Zur, 2007). While ethical or unavoidable dual relationships are technically boundary crossings, exploitative dual relationships, including sexual dual relationships, are definitely boundary violations. It is important to understand that therapeutic and ethical touch, like clinically appropriate boundary crossings, such as self-disclosure or making a home visit (done exclusively for clinical reasons and are not involved in a secondary relationship), are neither dual relationships nor unethical.

The difference between boundary crossings and boundary violations, when it comes to touch, often relates to the differences between sexual and non-sexual touch (Pope, Sonne, & Holroyd, 1993; Zur, 2007). Some differentiations between sexual and nonsexual touch in therapy focus on the areas touched (i.e., hand vs. genitals), others focus on whether the intent is to sexually arouse the client or the therapist, and yet others propose an encompassing view that “erotic touch” is any behavior that leads to sexual arousal (e.g., Brodsky, 1985). A few analytically oriented scholars take the extreme position that – in the context of transference – even what attempts to be a nonsexual touch is almost inevitably sexual or erotic. (Gabbard, 1996; Wrye & Welles, 1994). However, this is a perspective that is almost exclusive to the pure, traditional psychoanalysis. Help with differentiation between sexual and nonsexual touch in therapy comes from one of the key studies that found correlations between nonsexual touch and sexual touch. The study showed that the sexual boundary violation was positively correlated, not with touch per se, but with the frequency that therapists touched clients of the opposite sex in comparison with the frequency of touch of clients of the same sex (Holroyd & Brodsky, 1980). The important conclusion of these findings was that therapists’ own attitudes towards touch and whether they tend to generally sexualize all forms of touch is the determining factor in whether they are likely to blur sexual and nonsexual forms of touch. Therefore the most productive preventative measure is probably good therapist education in appropriate use of therapeutic use of touch.

As was noted above, professional attitudes towards therapeutic boundaries in general has shifted significantly during the end of the last century and the beginning of the 21st century. An increase in the number of publications that associate boundary crossings and touch to increased therapeutic effectiveness has linked them to the most commonly practiced theoretical orientations, such as cognitive, cognitive-behavioral and humanistic psychotherapies (Williams, 1997). Illustrating the shift in mainstream psychology and counseling towards more context-based and less rigid attitudes towards boundaries is the flexibility advocated by American Psychological Association’s (APA) revised code of ethics of 2016 and similar changes included in the American Counseling Association’s (ACA) code of ethics of 2014. Just as telling is the fact that American Psychological Association (APA) and American Counseling Association (ACA) have published several texts in the beginning of the 21st century that have taken a clear, flexible and context-based stance in regard to therapeutic boundaries (i.e., Herlihy & Corey, 2006; Knapp & VandeCreek, 2006; Schank & Skovholt, 2006; Zur, 2007). Additionally, during this period there was an increased realization of the potentially immense clinical usefulness or benefit of ethical professional touch (Field, 1998, 2003; Hunter and Struve, 1998; May, 2005; Smith, et al., 1998; Young, 2005). In the beginning of the new century there were a few influential papers that re-introduced the importance and clinical utility of touch within the analytic context (i.e., Fosshage, 2000; LaPierre, 2003; Schore, 2003; Toronto, 2001). The inception of the United States Association of Body Psychotherapy Journal in 2002 has been a significant additional continuous and extensive contribution to the professional literature on clinical, ethical and legal issues that pertain to touch in therapy. An additional rich resource of body psychotherapy has become available through European Body-Psychotherapists (2006).

Top of Page
 

The Standard Of Care: What It Is And What It Is Not

The standard of care is one of the most important constructs in medicine and mental health. It guides practitioners in their practices, provides a minimum professional standard and is an essential element in malpractice suits and hearings of state licensing boards. Because the standard of care is both important and elusive, it is the subject of much debate and controversy. Surprisingly, there is no one national or universally accepted standard of care that can be found in any agreed upon text. The standard of care is primarily determined in courts by juries, judges and by licensing board hearings, which often rely on the testimony of expert witnesses. In these hearings attorneys on both sides routinely present conflicting expert testimonies about the standard of care (Gutheil, 1998; Hedges, Hilton, Hilton, & Caudill, 1997). The fact that there are hundreds of different psychotherapeutic orientations (Lambert, 1991) and as many different types of settings, communities, cultures and subcultures, makes the concept of a psychotherapeutic standard of care extremely complicated and controversial (Caudill, 2004; Williams, 1997). It seems that beyond “do no harm,” “do not engage in sexual relationships with current clients” and “preserve clients’ dignity and protect their privacy when possible,” there is very little agreement on what falls within the accepted understanding of standard of care.

The standard of care is a legal term and has been defined as the customary professional practice in the community. It describes the qualities and conditions that prevail, or should prevail, in a particular mental health service and that a reasonable or average practitioner follows. Most commonly, the standard is defined in legal terms as, “that degree of care which a reasonably prudent person would exercise in the same or similar circumstances” (Black, 1990, p. 1405). As a legal term, the standard of care is subject to state laws and, accordingly, the official definition of the standard of care varies somewhat from state to state. Massachusetts case law, for example, defines the standard of care as, “the average reasonable practitioner at that time and under the circumstances and taken into account the advances in the field” (Gutheil, 1998, p. 44). The standard of care is thus largely a standard of reasonable care and thus is a professional duty of psychotherapists to their clients once the therapist-patient relationship has been established (Simon, 2001). Several scholars emphasized that the standard is based on community and professional standards, and as such, professionals are held to the same standard as others of the same profession or discipline with comparable qualifications in similar localities (Bersoff, 2003; Caudill, 2004; Doverspike, 1999; Woody, 1998).

It is very important to understand that the standard of care is a minimum and reasonable standard. It is neither an ideal standard nor a standard of perfection (Gutheil, 1998). It calls on practitioners to act in a reasonable, average or “good enough” manner rather than in ideal or perfect ways. An error in judgment or simply making a common, careless mistake does not automatically put a therapist’s actions below the standard of care (Simon, 2001). However, making a careless mistake or several careless mistakes that probably would not have been made by reasonable practitioners does put a therapist below the standard of care. Gross negligence, which is an extreme departure from the standard of care, has been differentiated from a simple departure from the standard and from common or normal, unavoidable mistakes or errors in judgment.

Basic Elements of the Standard of Care
The standard of care is derived from the following six elements: State law; Licensing board regulations; Professional organization codes of ethics; Case laws; Consensus of the professionals; and Consensus in the community.

1. Statutes: Each state has many statutes, such as Child Abuse, Elder Abuse, Domestic Violence Reporting and other laws. If the statute mandates that therapists do not act or should act in a certain way, such as reporting a suspicion of child abuse, acting against that prohibition, or neglecting to so act, is clearly below the statutory standard of care.

2. Licensing board regulations: In most states there are extensive regulations governing many aspects of mental health practices. These often include rules for continuing education, supervision, etc. Some licensing boards have adopted numerous additional regulations that range from how to engage in e-counseling or telehealth, to how to respond to a client who discloses in therapy that he or she had sexual relations with a former therapist. In all states and in The District of Columbia, there are strict regulations against a therapist having sexual relationships with a current psychotherapy client.

3. Ethical codes of professional associations: The codes of ethics of professional associations are another important component of the general standard of care. However, they are also controversial in regard to the standard of care. In most situations codes of ethics of professional organizations apply to members and non-members of the professional association. APA (2016), NASW (2017), ACA (2014) and AAMFT (2015) ethical principles apply to all licensed psychologists, social workers, counselors and marriage and family therapists, respectively, regardless of whether they are members of the organizations or not, unless there is a state law or board regulation stating otherwise. Some states adopted other professional organizations’ codes of ethics as their standard. An example is California Board of Behavioral Sciences (CA-BBS), which regulates California Marriage and Family Therapists (MFTs), adopted the California Marriage and Family Therapists Association (CAMFT) code of ethics as their standard rather than AAMFT code of ethics.

Translating most codes of ethics, licensing board regulations or using them to clarify the standard of care can be a complex and challenging task. The codes are generally not specific about which behaviors are prohibited, and most codes include aspirational goals, which must be viewed differently than the enforceable ones (Bersoff, 1994; Fleer, 2000; Williams, 2003). While many state licensing boards have adopted the codes of ethics of major professional organizations as their enforceable guidelines, the APA and other codes of ethics are not intended to be a basis of civil liability. In other words, the codes of ethics are not supposed to be simply equated with the standard of care, which is the basis for civil liability.

Another area of uncertainty is whether practitioners who practice in a more specialized field, or present themselves as specialists, are to be held not only to national professional organization ethical standards (i.e., AAMFT, ACA, APA, NASW, NBCC) but also to standards put forth by their specialty (i.e., child custody evaluation, forensic psychology), specialized professional association (i.e., US Association of Body Psychotherapists (USABP), Academy of Sports Psychology) or institution they are closely affiliated with (i.e., Jung Institute of San Francisco, Gestalt Institute of Los Angeles, The Reichian Institute of Sacramento).

4. Case law: Case law is one of the cornerstones of the standard of care. No case is more famous for having created a duty (to warn) for psychotherapists than the Tarasoff decision of the California Supreme Court in Tarasoff v. Regents of the University California (1976).

5. Consensus of the professionals: In a field that is comprised of hundreds of therapeutic orientations and even more jurisdictions, consensus among professionals is hard to come by. Thus, it follows that consensus among professionals is a rather vague aspect of the standard of care. It is primarily derived from a wide range of diverse professional publications (Younggren & Gottlieb, 2004), professional association guidelines and presentations at professional conferences. An additional complexity of this part of the standard is what has been called the “respectable minority.” This doctrine may apply when there is significant support for a certain type of treatment of a certain disorder, or if the scientific or research support of the technique is not well established (Reid, 1998, Simon, 2001).

6. Consensus in the community: While some scholars emphasize the general, unified or global aspects of the standard of care across settings, others emphasize the importance of community, local culture and setting in determining the standard. Following the latter line of thought, consequently, different setting and communities, which abide by different cultural customs and values, have different standards. For example the exchange of gifts and attending ceremonies and rituals are normal and expected in Hispanic or American Indian communities but not necessarily in an upper class suburban clinic (Lazarus & Zur, 2002; Zur, 2001). Complex dual relationships between therapists and clients are inherent, and, in fact, mandated by law, in the military and are common in rural areas but are infrequent in urban areas (Zur, 2007).

What the Standard of Care is Not
The standard of care has often been viewed in several inaccurate ways, some of which have had a direct implication in understanding the relationship between touch and the standard of care. Following is a non-exhaustive list of what the standard of care is not:

1. It is not a standard of perfection. It is the standard based on the average practitioner and on reasonable or “good enough” actions. Caudill (2004) describes it as a ‘C’ student’s standard. Simply making a common or ordinary mistake or common error in judgment does not automatically put a therapist’s actions below the standard of care (Simon, 2001).

2. It is not an either/or standard. Compliance or non-compliance with the standard of care has gradations or shades of deviation from the standard. Most commonly, three terms have been used to describe the range of practices: gross negligence; simple departure from the standard of care; and mistakes or errors in judgment. Gross negligence has often been defined as an extreme departure from the ordinary standard of practice in the community. Gross negligence often involves a pattern of systematic and/or extreme departure from the minimum and reasonable standard of practice. Gross negligence is almost always one of the key components of malpractice suits and licensing board hearings. The next level, a simple departure from the standard of practice, has been called “ordinary negligence.” The third level, the most common one, is a simple mistake or error in judgment, which is an unavoidable part of human nature and of the practice of psychotherapy and does not constitute departure from the standard of practice.

3. It is not guided by risk management principles. One of the most significant errors by expert witnesses, attorneys, courts and licensing boards has been confusing the standard of care with risk management principles (Lazarus & Zur, 2002; Williams, 1997). While the standard is based on legal-professional-communal principles, risk management guidelines are primarily enforced to reduce the risk of malpractice accusations for therapists (Williams, 2003; Gutheil & Gabbard, 1993; Zur, 2007). While the standard of care focuses on what is good for the patient, risk management guidelines have come to focus, too often, on preemptive protection of therapists and reducing insurance companies’ financial liability.

4. It does not follow any particular therapeutic modality or theoretical orientation. The standard of care is theoretically blind and philosophically neutral. It is not based on psychiatric, biological, analytic or any other therapeutic modality or theoretical orientation. Attorneys and experts have often presented the psychoanalytic guidelines as the basis for the standard of care (Williams, 1997). Gutheil (1989) accurately pointed out: “It seems that professionals who belong to a school of thought that rejects the idea of transference, behaviorists, or psychiatrists who provide only drug treatment, are being held to a standard of care they do not acknowledge” (p. 31).

5. It is not determined by outcome. Interventions by therapists who do not violate the law or board regulations and utilize “good enough” decision-making processes are most likely to fall within the standard of care, even if the outcome is negative. An unfortunate outcome, such as suicide, divorce or depression, does not necessarily translate to substandard care (Baerger, 2001; Simon, 2001).

6. It is not permanent or fixed. The standard of care is a dynamic standard that continues to evolve over time. Obviously, new statutes and new case laws change the standard. Then, as more practitioners practice in new or modified ways, the standard changes, too. HIPAA law is an example of how new regulations, significantly, impact the standard of care (Zur, 2005). The continuously revised professional ethics codes, publication of new research findings, new practice guidelines or new theoretical breakthroughs all can affect the standard.

Top of Page
 

Touch In Psychotherapy

The importance of touch for human development, communicating, bonding and healing has been scientifically studied and documented for the last half century by culturally iconic figures, such as Bowlby (1952), Harlow (1971) and Montagu (1986) and more recently by Tiffany Field (1998, 2003). Ample research has demonstrated that tactile stimulation is extremely important for development and maintenance of physiological and psychological regulation in infants, children and adults (Field, 1998, 2003; Heller, 1997; LaPierre, 2006). Touch has been an essential part of ancient healing practices and is reported to have been an integral part of health care practices and medicine since the beginning of time (Levitan & Johnson, 1986; Smith, et al., 1998). In his seminal work, Touching: The Human Significance of the Skin, Ashley Montagu brings together a vast array of studies shedding light on the role of skin and physical touch in human development. He goes on to illuminate how the sensory system, the skin, is the most important organ system of the body. “Among all the senses,” Montagu states, “touch stands paramount” (1986, p. 17), and he concludes: “When the need for touch remains unsatisfied, abnormal behavior will result” (1986, p. 46). Indeed, touch deprivation has been consistently linked to aggression, delinquency, social isolation and depression in children and adults (Field, 2003).

Recent research has demonstrated that touch triggers a cascade of chemical responses, including a decrease in urinary stress hormones (i.e., cortisol, catecholamines, norepinephrine, epinephrine) and an increase in serotonin and dopamine levels (LaPierre, 2006). The shift in these bio-chemicals has been proven to decrease depression (Field, 2003). Touch is, obviously, good medicine. It also enhances the immune system by increasing natural killer cells and killer cell activity, balancing the ratio of cd4 cells and cd4/cd8 cells. The immune system’s cytotoxic capacity increases with touch, thus helping the body maintain its defense against pathogens (Field, 1998).

The utility of psychotherapeutic touch has been extensively documented. Generally, touch has been reported to effectively reduce stress, anxiety, dissociation and depression, and can be very effective in the treatment of Post Traumatic Stress Disorder. It has also been repeatedly reported that touch in therapy positively influences bonding between therapists and clients and increases the therapeutic alliance, the best predictor of positive therapeutic outcome. At this point we must differentiate between ‘therapy’ and psychotherapy’. While there are many therapies that legitimately involve touch, like physiotherapy and massage, they are not considered psychotherapy. Accordingly, the focus of this paper is on psychotherapy or counseling. Obviously, psychotherapy, per se, does not necessarily involve touch even though most therapists (85%) hug their clients rarely or sometimes (Pope, et. Al. 1987) and almost all shake hands with their clients (Smith, et al., 1998). Body psychotherapy defines itself as involving the potential for appropriate professional touch (Young, 2005) and, accordingly, most body psychotherapists are specifically trained to employ touch as part of psychotherapy.

While review of the literature of the effectiveness of touch in mental health services is beyond the scope of this paper, extensive reviews of the research on touch can be found in the works of Durana (1998), Field (2003), Heler (1997) Hunter & Struve (1998), May (2005), Marten (2006), McNeil-Haber (2004); Nordmarken and Zur (2004), Smith, et al., (1998) and Young (2005).

In this article touch refers to any physical contact occurring between a psychotherapist and a client or patient in the context of psychotherapy. Generally, there are four types of touch in psychotherapy: touch that is used as an adjunct to verbal psychotherapy, systematic touch that is used by specially trained body psychotherapists, and inappropriate touch. Following are detailed descriptions of the three types of touch in therapy.

The first type of touch ncludes touch employed as an adjunct to verbal psychotherapy. These forms of touch are intentionally and strategically used to enhance a sense of connection with the client and to sooth, greet, relax or reassure the client. Their use is also intended to reduce anxiety, slow heartbeat, physically and emotionally calm the client, or assist the client in moving out of a dissociative state. It also includes culturally appropriate touch. Therapeutic touch, in this context, most often includes a hug, light touch, handholding, or rubbing and the places of contact are usually on a client’s back, shoulder or arm. Based partly on formulations by Downey (2001), Nordmarken and Zur (2004), Smith, et al., (1998) and Zur (2007) these forms of touch may fall under the following categories:

  • Ritualistic or socially accepted gestures for greeting and goodbye or arrival and departure: These gestures figure significantly among most cultures and include handshakes, a greeting or farewell embrace and other culturally accepted gestures.
  • Conversational marker: This form of light touch on the arm, hand, back or shoulder is intended to make or highlight a point and can also take place at times of stillness, with the purpose of accentuating the therapist’s presence and conveying attention.
  • Consolatory touch: This important form of touch, holding the hands or shoulders of a client or providing a comforting hug, is most likely to enhance therapeutic alliance.
  • Reassuring touch: This form of touch is geared to encouraging and reassuring clients and usually involves a pat on the back or shoulder.
  • Playful touch: This form of touch, mostly of hand, shoulders or head, may take place while playing a game with a child or adolescent client.
  • Grounding or reorienting touch: This form of touch is intended to help clients reduce anxiety or dissociation by using touch to the hand or arm or by leading them to touch their own hand or arm.
  • Task-oriented touch: This involves touch that is merely ancillary to the task at hand, such as offering a hand to help someone stand up or bracing an arm around a client’s shoulders to keep the client from falling.
  • Corrective experience: This form of touch may involve the holding of an adult or rocking of a child by a therapist who practices forms of therapy that emphasize the importance of corrective experiences.
  • Instructional or modeling touch: Therapists may model how to touch or respond to touch by demonstrating a firm handshake, holding an agitated child or responding to unwanted touch.
  • Celebratory or congratulatory touch: The therapist may give a pat on the back or a congratulatory hug to a client who has achieved a goal.
  • Experiential touch: This form of touch usually takes place when the therapist conducts an experiential exercise, such as teaching gestures during assertiveness training or in family sculpturing, in which family members are asked to assume certain positions in relationship to each other.
  • Referential touch: This is often done in group or family therapy when the therapist lightly taps the arm or shoulder of a client, indicating that he or she can take a turn or be silent.
  • Inadvertent touch: This is touch that is unintentional, involuntary and unpremeditated, such as an inadvertent brush against a client by the therapist.
  • Touch intended to prevent a client from hurting him- or herself: This type of touch is intended to stop self-harming behaviors, such as head banging, self-hitting or self-cutting.
  • Touch intended to prevent someone from hurting another: This form of touch is intended to stop or restrain someone from hurting another person, as sometimes happens in family, couple or group therapy, or when working with extremely volatile clients.
  • Touch in therapist’s self-defense: This form of touch is used by a therapist to physically defend himself or herself from the assault of a violent client by using self-defense techniques that restrain clients with minimum force. – – – – (Zur, 2007, p. 173-174)

The second type of touch in therapy includes:

  • Therapeutic touch by body psychotherapists: This is different than the use of touch as an adjunct to verbal psychotherapy. Most somatic and body psychotherapists, who are specially trained in these modalities, regularly use touch as part of their theoretically prescribed clinical interventions and these psychotherapies can include Reichian (LaTorre, 2005; Reich, 1972), Bioenergetics (Lowen, 1958), Somatic Experiencing (Levine & Frederick, 1997), Rubenfeld Synergy, Hakomi, Biodynamic Psychotherapy, Biosynthesis, amongst the many other modalities described by Barshop (2005), Aposhyan (2004) and others. The history of body psychotherapy has been reviewed by Young (1997) and the definition articulated by United States Association of Body Psychotherapy (USABP) (2006) and European Association of Body-Psychotherapy (EABP) (2006).

The third type includes inappropriate forms of touch,
and is in contrast to the aforementioned forms of touch. The following three forms of touch in psychotherapy are unethical, considered as boundary violations and, depending on the state, often illegal (Smith, et al., 1998; Zur, 2007). They are counter-clinical and should always be avoided. They include:

  • Sexual touch.
  • Hostile or violent touch.
  • Punishing touch.

While this paper focuses on touch that is initiated by the therapist, it is quite usual for clients to initiate touch. Most common client initiated touch is a handshake. McNeil-Harber (2004) discussed touch that is initiated by children-patients usually differentiates between aggressive or oversexualized, inappropriate touch and appropriate touch. When a client initiates or requests touch, the therapist must use his or her clinical judgment to ascertain whether providing or withholding touch is ethical, and if it is clinically advantageous in each therapeutic situation.

In summary, touch has been indisputably important for human development, bonding and healing. Touch is being extensively employed in a variety of ways as an adjunct to verbal psychotherapy and in many long established and well researched body psychotherapy modalities.

Top of Page
 

Therapeutic Touch And The Standard Of Care

With the demystification of the standard of care and the summary of the general issues involved in psychotherapy, it is now appropriate for a discussion of the specific application of the elements that comprise the standard of care to non-sexual touch in psychotherapy. As sexual touch between therapists and current clients is always unethical and illegal in most states, the discussion below, like the focus of this paper, is on non-sexual touch.

When it comes to the standard of care, it is very clear that there are neither statues nor licensing board regulations nor ethics codes of any major professional association that prohibit non-sexual, clinically appropriate touch. State and federal laws, licensing board regulations and professional organization codes of ethics do not even mention, to say the least, regulate or prohibit non-sexual, ethical therapeutic touch. As was noted above, state and federal laws, board regulations and codes of ethics are all modality neutral. Therefore, the applications of touch as an adjunct to verbal psychotherapy (e.g., supportive touch at times of distress, appropriate hug at the end of a session) are treated by federal and state laws, licensing boards or codes of ethics no differently than any other appropriate boundary crossing (e.g., self-disclosure, gifts). Similarly, somatic and body psychotherapy interventions (e.g., Bioenergetics, Orgonomy, Somatic Experiencing) are treated by federal and state laws, licensing boards and codes of ethics no differently than any other therapeutic technique (e.g., Cognitive-behavioral, Gestalt).

Evaluating the issue of case law is highly complicated when it comes to touch in therapy. The main reason for this is that experts testifying for plaintiffs have often erroneously argued that non-sexual touch is likely to lead to sexual touch and, therefore, is below the standard of care. (Zur, 2007) Another common erroneous argument has been the fact that patients who have reported they were aroused by a therapist’s touch meant that the touch-intervention was below the standard of care (Williams, 2000). While the former argument is based on the fallacious slippery slope argument, the latter one erroneously claims that therapists are to be judged by the outcome of treatment rather than by the process of decision-making and adherence to laws and regulations. To my knowledge there has not been any case law that mandated the avoidance of all non-sexual touch in therapy.

The standard of care element that refers to consensus among professionals is highly relevant to therapeutic touch. As was cited above, there is a vast body of literature that supports the importance of touch as an adjunct to verbal psychotherapy (see summaries in Fields, 2003; Hunter & Struve, 1998; Smith, et al., 1998) and as a discipline of its own, as embodied in body psychotherapy. The “respectable minority” provision discussed above is also highly relevant to touch in therapy as it establishes that the many less established and less researched varieties of body psychotherapy, and the many forms of ethical touch, which is employed as an adjunct to verbal psychotherapy, do not necessary fall below the standard of care.

The part of the standard of care that states that it is also bound by community norms is also applicable to touch in psychotherapy. Practicing in certain Latin, African American, French or Jewish communities or rehabilitation centers often involves culturally or community-appropriate touch between therapists and clients. A full-body hug, or a peck on both cheeks (“European kiss”) is often the culturally appropriate greeting ritual within these communities or settings. This element of the standard of care clearly establishes that different settings and communities, which abide by different cultural customs and values, often have different standards including different therapeutic standards of care in regard to touch. For example, extensive physical touch may be employed in adventure therapy or sport psychology. The community standard is also applied where therapists are working in certain settings that focus on somatic or body psychotherapy. For example, therapists who practice in training institutions that focus on Reichian therapy or Somatic Experiencing are likely to use these touch-based techniques extensively. To comply with these professional standards, special informed consent forms in regard to touch may be asked for from clients such settings.

As was discussed above, the standard is neither guided by risk management principles of avoiding touching a client beyond a handshake, nor by the physically distanced approach of the psychoanalytically based modalities. Therefore, risk management and analytic yardsticks are not applicable to appropriate, ethical and clinically driven therapeutic touch.

Highly relevant to the issue of touch is the fact that the standard of care is not determined by outcome. Like the example above, where a client’s suicide does not necessary mean that the therapist operated below the standard of care, a client’s sexual feelings in response to a therapist’s touch does not necessarily mean that the therapist was engaged in sexual touch. What is relevant to the standard of care is the therapist’s clinical rationale for the touch, the client’s consent, as well as the clinically appropriate evaluation of the impact of the touch and the therapist’s appropriate follow up. Also relevant is the therapist’s ethical decision-making process that led to the touch and understanding how the touch fits within the original treatment plan. In other words, the sheer fact that a client felt sexually aroused does not mean that the therapist operated below the standard of care. As with any other intervention, it is the responsibility of the therapist to conduct competent evaluation of the effect of the touch by observing the client, asking the client for feedback or by other means. If a therapist realizes that the touch resulted in unintended sexual arousal, it is his or her responsibility to attend to that in a clinically appropriate manner. This may include discussing it with the client, stopping or changing the touch, or other clinically appropriate responses.

Ethics Of Touch
The question of the ethics of touch has often been raised in relation to therapeutic touch. As with the APA Ethics Code (American Psychological Association, 2016), ethics codes of all major psychotherapy professional associations, such as American Association for Marriage and Family Therapists (AAMFT, 2015), American Counseling Association (ACA, 2014), and National Association of Social Workers, (NASW, 2017), neither specifically mention nor prohibit the use of appropriate, non-sexual touch in therapy. All psychotherapy professional codes of ethics view sexual touch with a current client as unethical. The answer to whether touch is ethical is simple and clear: clinically appropriate touch in psychotherapy is neither unethical nor below the standard of care.

Historically, unethical sexual touch in therapy received extensive attention (i.e., Pope, 1990; Pope, Sonne, & Holroyd, 1993; Rutter, 1989; Simon, 1994), but towards the turn of the 20th century and into the 21st century increased numbers of publications have attended to the ethics of touch (i.e., Durana, 1998; Herlihy & Corey, 2006; Hunter & Struve, 1998; Marten, 2006; McNeil-Haber, 2004; Nordmarken & Zur, 2004; Smith, et al., 1998; White, 2002; Young, 2005; Zur, 2007). These publications discuss the importance of taking into consideration client factors, such as history of abuse, gender, culture, attitude towards touch, presenting problem, as well as the setting of therapy, therapeutic modality employed, nature of the therapeutic relationship and therapist’s training, culture, gender and attitude towards touch. Appropriate use of consultation and client consent is also emphasized.

Obviously, the ethics of touch has received the most extensive coverage in the Ethical Guidelines of the U.S. Association of Body Psychotherapy (USABP, 2001). It clearly articulates the ethical guidelines for the use of touch in therapy, the importance of informed consent and concerns with respect, diversity, consultation, record keeping, treatment plans and many other pertinent issues for ethical touch in psychotherapy. Most body psychotherapists use actual touch as their primary tool in psychotherapy, and, therefore, an extra focus on the ethics of touch is called for in their therapist’s training (Caldwell, 1997; Durana, 1998; LaPierre, 2003; Phillips, 2002; Smith, et al., 1998; Young, 1997).

Partly in respond to testimonies by psychoanalytically oriented and risk management expert witnesses against boundary crossing in general, including testimonies disparaging clinically appropriate and ethical touch, APA Ethics Code of 2016 introduced a much-needed clarity to the issues when it provided a definition of “reasonable.” In the Introduction and Applicability section it states:

The modifiers used in some of the standards of this Ethics Code (e.g., reasonably, appropriate, potentially ) are included in the standards when they would (1) allow professional judgment on the part of psychologists, (2) eliminate injustice or inequality that would occur without the modifier, (3) ensure applicability across the broad range of activities conducted by psychologists, or (4) guard against a set of rigid rules that might be quickly outdated. As used in this Ethics Code, the term reasonable means the prevailing professional judgment of psychologists engaged in similar activities in similar circumstances, given the knowledge the psychologist had or should have had at the time. (p. 162)

Clearly, one of the intentions of this statement is this: no longer will courts and licensing boards, who define the standard of care, use any one particular modality or orientation as the yard stick, and by which measure interventions that are rooted in other disciplines: this means analytic or risk management principles legitimately may not apply to a body psychotherapy situation. The APA statement acknowledges that some clinical situation boundary crossings, such as gifts, bartering or dual relationships, may be appropriate, clinically beneficial and unavoidable. This statement is as relevant to touch in psychotherapy and clearly implies that the evaluation of the appropriateness of touch in therapy must be according to the ” . . . prevailing professional judgment of psychologists engaged in similar activities in similar circumstances . . .” which means the “prevailing professional judgment” of other psychologists who use touch with similar client populations and in similar settings. For the first time this paragraph actually makes it unethical for a testifying psychologist-expert to use psychoanalytic, psycho-pharmacological or other therapeutic orientations to determine that clinically appropriate and ethical touch, supported by other established orientations, is below the standard of care.

Top of Page
 

Risk Management Practices, Slippery Slope Claims, Sexualization Of Touch And Risk-Benefit Analysis

At the core of the risk management injunction against touch in psychotherapy is an assumption about a ‘slippery slope.’ This refers to the idea that failure to adhere to hands-off, rigid standards will most likely lead to therapist-client sexual relationships. This process is described by Gabbard (1994) as follows: ” . . . the crossing of one boundary without obvious catastrophic results (making) it easier to cross the next boundary” (p. 284). This fear-based view has been most dominant in the discussion of employing or incorporating touch in psychotherapy as it asserts that a pat on the back, hand-holding, non-sexual supportive or a greeting hug are all just the first downhill steps towards inevitable deterioration of ethical conduct and towards sexual relationships. Pope (1990), whose endorsement of the slippery slope idea has significantly contributed to its popularity, stated: ” . . . non-sexual dual relationships, while not unethical and harmful per se, foster sexual dual relationships” (p. 688). Similarly, Strasburger, Jorgenson, & Sutherland (1992) conclude, “Obviously, the best advice to therapists is not to start (down) the slippery slope, and to avoid boundary violations or dual relationships with patients.” (p. 547-548). Also in agreement is Simon (1991), who decrees: “The boundary violation precursors of therapist-patient sex can be as psychologically damaging as the actual sexual involvement itself” (p. 614). These poignant restrictive statements summarize the slippery slope idea and its derivative risk management stance that the chance of exploitation and harm is significantly reduced or nullified by simply refraining from engaging in any boundary crossing, including any form of non-sexual touch, regardless of its clinical effectiveness. This is a false argument and contains an unproven non-sequitur.

A careful review of the ‘slippery slope’ argument reveals that it is founded primarily on the (somewhat paranoid) assumption that any boundary crossing, however trivial, inevitably leads to boundary violations and sex (Lazarus & Zur, 2002; Zur, 2007). The slippery slope argument claims to get support from the fact that most sexual exploitations of clients have started with non-sexual or ethical boundary crossing. Whilst it may be true that abusive therapists might have introduced themselves with a handshake, it does not follow that this leads to sexual relationships, and a vastly significant number of therapists have also introduced themselves with a handshake, to no ill effect.

Furthermore, to assert that self-disclosure is likely to lead to social relationships, that an appropriate hug is most likely to end with intercourse, or accepting a gift is the first step towards inevitable inappropriate business relationships is irrational and illogical. Sonne (1994) discusses how a therapist and client who are sport teammates can easily move their relationship to encompass activities, such as carpooling or drinking. She concludes that, “With the blurring of the expected functions and responsibilities of the therapist and client comes the breakdown of the boundaries of the professional relationship itself” (p. 338). Along these lines Woody (1998) asserts, “In order to minimize the risk of sexual conduct, policies must prohibit a practitioner from having any contact with the client outside the treatment context and must preclude any type of dual relationships” (p. 188). The risk management literature is saturated with articles and books describing therapists’ behaviors (e.g. self-disclosure, hugs, home visits, socializing, longer sessions, lunching, exchanging gifts, walks, playing in recreational leagues) that the authors contend are precursors to or are on the slippery slope to sexual or other harmful dual relationships (Borys & Pope, 1989; Craig, 1991; Koocher & Keith-Spiegel 1998; Lakin, 1991; Pope, 1990; Pope & Vasquez, 2007; Rutter, 1989; St. Germaine, 1996).

The belief in the slippery slope is a part of the more widespread cultural and professional problem, which includes risk management experts’ tendency to sexualize boundaries in general and touch in particular (Dineen, 1996; Zur, 2007). This so-called ‘prudence’ is akin to prudery, where the sin lies more in the eye of the beholder than in the mind of the enactor. It must be challenged wherever it tries to circumscribe. But there is also a wider issue, why do touch taboos exist and why is there such touch illiteracy. Field (2003) notes that social attitudes to touch have changed, from the ‘laying-on’ of hands (common in the Bible, Middle Ages and still found in some religions), to touch becoming sexual in the 19th century (as everything was, even the sight of a woman’s ankle), to touch being now “more associated with criminality is increasingly frequent court cases on sexual harassment, sexual abuse, child-care hysteria and kindergarten and lower grade teachers’ avoidance of any form of touch beyond a handshake.

When it comes to touch in therapy, the slippery slope idea basically claims that there is no meaningful differentiation between non-sexual touch and sexual touch because one inevitably or very likely leads to the other. Again, there is little real evidence put forward for this claim. In a critical examination of the slippery slope argument, Zur (2000) reflects that to assert, as most proponents of the slippery slope idea do, that self-disclosure, a home visit, a hug or accepting a gift are actions likely to lead to sex is like saying that doctors’ visits cause death because most people see a doctor before they die. Lazarus calls this thinking “an extreme form of syllogistic reasoning” (1994, p. 257). We learn in school that sequential statistical relationships (correlations) cannot simply be translated into causal ones. Despite the popularity of the term, the ‘slippery slope’ is a paranoid, baseless and illogical construct claiming that any deviation from risk management or rigid analytic guidelines is likely to lead to harm, exploitation and sex.

While all risk management and most ethics texts appropriately emphasize the important risk-benefits analysis for touch or any other clinical intervention, very few address the risk-benefits analysis of “in-action” or the avoidance of certain interventions (Zur, 2007). While risk-management focus is, obviously, on risk avoidance and most ethical decision-making processes emphasize the risk-benefits analysis of touching, rarely mentioned is the equally important risk-benefit analysis of avoiding touch at all cost. Along these lines the author has reflected on his experience of avoidance of touch at all cost:

For example, I have been working with a woman who, 10 years prior to our first session, lost her infant son in an automobile accident. In an emergency appointment with a psychiatrist right after the death of her son, as she sobbed uncontrollably, she begged him to hold her. He refused, citing something about professional boundaries. Instead, he prescribed Valium. Eight years later, addicted to Valium and alcohol, she began therapy with me. After an intense few months of therapy, we visited her son’s grave. It was the first time she had visited the grave. There we stood, holding each other and both weeping as she finally started facing her baby’s death and grieving for him and for her years lost in drugged denial. While the psychiatrist followed risk management guidelines to perfection, he also may have inflicted immense harm. Did he sacrifice his humanity and the core of his professional being, to heartless protocol? (Nordmarken & Zur, 2004)

Risk-benefit analysis of actions or inactions brings to the forefront the contexts of therapy (i.e., client factors, setting, therapy and therapist factors). Such risk-benefits must be included in treatment planning for the use of touch or any other interventions. The rarely acknowledged fact is that all clinical interventions also contain risk. As a matter of fact, any human action as well as any human inaction is associated with some level of risk. Therefore, a thorough risk-benefit analysis does not simply reject boundary crossings, such as touch, because it involves risk, instead they invite therapists to ask the question, “Do these risks outweigh the benefits?” or “Are these risks justified?” Therapists must always take into consideration that they can actually do harm through inaction and the avoidance of touch in the attempt to avoid harm (Fay, 2002; Lazarus & Zur, 2002).

When it comes to risk management in regard to touch, the question then becomes, “what can be done to reduce any inherent risks to a reasonable and appropriate level?” The ‘solution’ to the ‘problem’ (which exists in a very small minority of therapists, and thus the risk is very small) is not to restrict all therapists by penalizing codes, but simply to ensure they are educated better and aware of appropriate boundaries, and perhaps even supervise them a little more regularly.

There is an additional sociological question, “Why are behaviors and interventions, such as touch, that are known to be clinically helpful, as well as very natural elements of human interaction, being looked at as suspicious and driven underground?” The answer lies partially in the concept, practice and teaching of defensive medicine or risk management. Reflecting on the analytic touch taboo, Lapierre asserts, “From this perspective, the touch taboo and the resulting touch illiteracy limit our psychotherapeutic horizons and rob us of effective, perhaps critical, forms of clinical reparation interventions and interactive couple and caregiver education” (2003, p. 5). This paper demonstrates that from a standard of care point of view ethical touch, which is based on a thorough risk-benefit analysis and is a result of a sound ethical decision making process, inevitably falls within the standard of care.

Top of Page
 

Touch In Context

The clinical application of touch in psychotherapy can only be understood within the context of the therapy. Accordingly, whether therapeutic touch falls within the standard of care, or not, can also be understood within the context that it is employed. Touch, when viewed through the prism of client factors, therapeutic setting, therapeutic orientation, therapeutic relationship and therapist factors, can have radically different contextual meanings (Hedges, et al., 1997; Koocher & Keith-Spiegel, 1998; Phillips, 2002; Smith, et al., 1998; Young, 2005; Zur, 2007).

Former APA president and leading ethicist Gerry Koocher provides a vivid example of how professionals tend prematurely to judge touch and other boundary crossings without taking the context into consideration.

On occasion I tell my students and professional audiences that I once spent an entire psychotherapy session holding hands with a 26-year-old woman together in a quiet darkened room. That disclosure usually elicits more than a few gasps and grimaces. When I add that I could not bring myself to end the session after 50 minutes and stayed with the young woman holding hands for another half hour, and when I add the fact that I never billed for the extra time, eyes roll.Then, I explain that the young woman had cystic fibrosis with severe pulmonary disease and panic-inducing air hunger. She had to struggle through three breaths on an oxygen line before she could speak a sentence. I had come into her room, sat down by her bedside, and asked how I might help her. She grabbed my hand and said, “Don’t let go.” When the time came for another appointment, I called a nurse to take my place. By this point in my story most listeners, who had felt critical of or offended by the “hand holding,” have moved from an assumption of sexualized impropriety to one of empathy and compassion. (2006, p. xxii)

Following are descriptions of the five factors that can help to define the relationship to touch in the context of therapy.

Client Factors
This factor includes client’s age, gender, presenting problem, diagnosis, personality, personal touch history, culture and class. They are all highly relevant to the meaning and potential healing effect of touch in therapy. What is particularly appropriate and effective with one client may be clinically inappropriate and even damaging with another. Letting a young child jump into the therapist’s lap in the midst of family therapy may be very appropriate, but it is generally not permissible with an adult client. Reaching out gently and respectfully to hold the hand of a grieving mother may not have the intended positive effect if the same is done in early stages of therapy with a survivor of sexual abuse. The client’s past experiences with touch are important and so are their present attitudes towards touch. Elements of personal space are defined within a culture and affect the interpretation of therapeutic touch. In this context a therapist’s touch, or lack of touch, may be seen as distant, respectful or invasive depending on the socialization, culture and experience of the individual client (Aponte & Wohl, 2000; Smith, et al., 1998; Sue & Sue, 2003).

Gender issues are also extremely important in understanding the context of touch. Touch in psychotherapy occurs between therapists of both sexes and their female and male clients as well as same-sex therapist-client dyads (Brodsky, 1985). Research has confirmed that women respond more positively to touch than do men (Hunter & Struve, 1998). From birth, American females receive more affectionate touch from males, and females and are given greater permission to touch either gender and be touched by either gender. They are more likely to have and expect a broader repertoire of touch, and they are less likely than men to perceive sexual intent in men when touched by them (Downey, 2001; Smith, et al., 1998). The use of touch with survivors of childhood trauma has been much debated. Whereas some authors assert that touch in any form should never be used with this population, many others agree that the clinically appropriate and ethical use of touch with survivors of childhood abuse, when applied cautiously, can be invaluable in helping them heal and recover from their traumatic experiences. The concern is that there is a possibility that touch used with these clients may recreate, evoke or re-traumatize previous client-experienced dynamics of victimization (Lawry, 1998). Cornell (1997) stated that once a strong therapeutic alliance has been formed, “the use of touch will evoke, address and hopefully help correct such historical experiences and distortion” (p. 33). What seems to be of the highest importance is that the client must want to be touched and understand the concepts of choice and personal empowerment before it is clinically or ethically appropriate to begin the use of touch in session. Research has also found that sexually abused clients were more likely to attribute a corrective or educative role to touch in therapy than were non-abused clients. Of these clients, 71% reported that appropriate touch repaired self-esteem, trust and a sense of their own power or agency, especially in setting limits and asking for what they need (Smith, et al., 1998).

Consistent with the pattern in the general culture, therapists tend to touch young clients more often than they do their adult clients, and female therapists touch child clients more often than do male therapists (Hunter & Struve, 1998). Research has demonstrated that when the staff of an adolescent treatment program modeled nonsexual, nonviolent touch and incorporated physical contact as an acceptable aspect of the milieu, the adolescents demonstrated a marked decrease in violent and sexual behaviors (Dunne, Bruggen, & O’Brien, 1982). Touch is usually contraindicated for clients who are actively paranoid, hostile and aggressive or who implicitly or explicitly demand touch (Durana, 1998). Most people experience some diminution in physical faculties and perceptual skills as they age, but the sense of touch generally remains intact and is valued as increasingly important as a source of contact and communication. The soothing, affirming experience of touch is most important at the beginning and end of one’s life and generous, nurturing touch can gently facilitate the process of aging and dying with dignity (Hollinger, 1986).

Setting Factor
The setting of therapy is profoundly important in evaluating the efficacy and meaning of touch. Some settings, such as prisons, are likely to restrict touch, whereas clinics for children or hospice are likely to encourage it. Obviously, sport psychology, adventure therapy, such as rope courses or flying trapeze, and adolescent programs that involve sports and camping, often involve extensive forms of touch (Zur, 2007). Practicing in different cultural milieus is likely to result in different attitudes and use of touch. Latino or Middle Eastern clients are likely to endorse and expect physical touch more than Northern European, Japanese or North American clients (Smith, et al., 1998). With levels of class and authority, it often moves from higher to lower; that is, a higher ranking individual may initiate touch of a subordinate but not vice versa. The same is true of male-to-female interaction in some societies (Halbrook & Duplechin, 1994). Touch, as an aspect of group therapy or in a therapeutic community, is probably more accepted and more often found than in one-to-one therapy.

Therapeutic Relationship (Therapeutic Alliance) Factors
The therapeutic relationship between therapists and clients, or the nature and quality of the therapeutic alliance, are among the most important factors determining the potential efficacy of the use of touch in therapy. A therapist-client relationship of trust and of long duration is more likely to create a familiar and safe context for effective use of touch in therapy. In contrast, a shorter or conflictual or confrontational relationship is less likely to be conducive to it. The relationship between touch and the therapeutic alliance seems to be bidirectional, as appropriate and “in-tune” touch significantly enhances positive therapeutic alliance (Horton, et al., 1995; Smith, et al., 1998) and, in return, creates a further atmosphere of trust and the possibility of the further use of clinically appropriate touch. Given that most research studies indicate that the therapeutic alliance is one of the most significant factors in respect of efficacy, one can then argue that, where appropriate and enhancing, touch can be seen as a significant factor in promoting the alliance and thus the efficacy of the therapy. It may even follow that ‘lack of touch’ might diminish efficacy and thus standard of care.

Therapeutic Orientation
As with any boundary consideration, therapeutic orientation or modality is exceptionally relevant in the clinical usefulness of touch in therapy. Body psychotherapists with clinical orientation, such as Reichian (Reich, 1972) or Bioenergetics (Lowen, 1976), often use touch as their primary tool in psychotherapy. In contrast, most traditional psychoanalysts are generally opposed to any form of touch in therapy (Menninger, 1958; Smith, et al., 1988; Wolberg, 1967). Generally, humanistically oriented therapies are more likely to endorse appropriate, non-erotic touch as they view it as an enhancement of the therapist-client connection (Hunter & Struve, 1998; Williams 1997). Rogers (1970) discussed the value of touch and specifically described how he soothed clients by holding, embracing and kissing them. Gestalt therapy incorporates numerous forms of touch as an integral part of therapy (Perls, 1973). Gestalt practitioners place a special importance on nonverbal communication and nonverbal intervention. Unfortunately, gestalt practices in the 1960s and early 1970s, under Perls’s leadership, went too far and at times included unethical sexual touch in conjunction with therapy (Hunter & Struve, 1998). Family therapists, including Satir (1972), often use touch as an element of engaging clients in therapy (Holub & Lee, 1990). Behavioral and cognitive-behavioral therapists are likely to incorporate touch or any boundary crossing into therapy if it fits with their interventions, such as modeling or reinforcement (Zur, 2007). Orientations, such as feminist and group therapy, support the clinically appropriate use of touch (Milakovitch, 1993; Williams, 1997). A few modern analysts, such as Fosshage (2000), have differed with mainstream analytic doctrine and advocate the incorporation of clinically responsible use of touch in psychoanalytical and psychodynamically oriented therapies.

Consistent with the theoretical literature, Holroyd and Brodsky (1977) found that humanistic psychologists were more likely to engage in non-erotic touch than those of other orientations. Similarly, Pope, et al., (1987) reported that therapists of differing theoretical orientations have very different beliefs about the effect and practice of touching clients. They reported that 30% of humanistic therapists indicated that non-erotic hugging, kissing and affectionate touching might frequently benefit clients in psychotherapy. In contrast, only 6% of psychodynamic therapists indicated the same. Whereas most psychodynamic therapists thought touch could be easily misunderstood, humanistic therapists did not share this view. Similarly, Milakovitch (1993) compared therapists who use or do not use touch and reported that therapists who use touch are likely to subscribe to a humanistic theoretical orientation, whereas therapists who do not use touch usually subscribe to a psychodynamic orientation. Clients choosing those therapists might have similar differences.

Therapist Factors
Therapists’ culture, age and professional socializations are likely to affect their utilization of clinical touch. Older therapists were professionally socialized to practice with less fear of boundary crossing, are not trained in risk management practices and are more likely to use touch more casually than younger ones whose training included much more focus on risk management and defensive medicine (Williams, 1977). Therapists’ own cultural background is very likely to affect their personal comfort with touch and, therefore, its use in the clinical settings. Milakovitch (1993) compared therapists who touch and those who do not touch and found that besides the therapeutic orientation factor, therapists who touch obviously value touch in therapy and believe that gratifying the need to be touched is important. Therapists who do not touch believe that gratifying the need to be touched is detrimental to therapy and the client. Unlike therapists who do not touch, therapists who touch were more likely to be touched by their own therapists and had supervisors and professors who believe in the legitimacy of touch as a therapeutic tool. Therapists who touch were more likely to experience body psychotherapies as clients than therapists who do not touch.

Gender of therapists (and clients) seems to impact the use of non-erotic touch. Stake and Oliver (1991) found that female psychologists reported more touching of female than male clients. Male psychologists, on the other hand, reported more touching of male clients on the shoulders, arm, hand or knee, but more touching of female clients in ways such as hugging, holding hands, or touching face, hair or neck. These findings seem consistent with Holroyd and Brodsky’s (1977) finding that non-erotic touching occurs more frequently in female dyads than male dyads.

Top of Page
 

Demonstrating Compliance With The Standard Of Care

Compliance with the standard of care, in general, as well as with touch issues means that therapists have acted in a professionally reasonable manner and followed community and professional standards as have others of the same profession or discipline with comparable qualifications in similar situations. Due to the professional and public concern with therapeutic touch, demonstrating compliance is very important. One of the primary ways for therapists to demonstrate compliance with the standard of care is accomplished primarily by means of documentation in clinical records (Caudill, 2004; Hedges, 2000; Gutheil, 1998). Good records go hand in hand with quality care.

At the minimum, records for each client, couple or family should include: Diagnosis impression (does not need to be a DSM diagnosis, it can be developmental, familial or other impressions), initial assessment of mental status, details of the presenting problem, relevant biographical background information, treatment planning, including rationale for treatment, and revised treatment plans, as necessary, progress notes and termination notes. When therapists choose not to use widely used, mainstream or standard interventions, they must articulate their clinical rationale for their choice of treatment and demonstrate their awareness and consideration of different treatment options. Extra documentation is often required in cases of emergencies, crisis intervention, violence and abuse situations, mandated reporting, extensive touch, dual relationships and abrupt termination. Signed informed consents might be considered as more important when it comes to body psychotherapy practices and other therapies that employ touch extensively. Finally, consultations on relevant clinical, legal and ethical cases should be utilized when necessary and documented as part of the records (Younggren & Gottlieb, 2004). Consultation with experts is one of the best ways to establish that the standard of care was met (Younggren & Gottlieb, 2004). Such consultations with experts, or regular supervision, allows the psychotherapist to demonstrate that the clinical intervention he or she is engaged in is reported to be similar to what other reasonable psychotherapists would do under similar circumstances.

One way for psychotherapists to evaluate if their conduct is within the standard of care is to ask themselves several questions, such as: Does my conduct violate state or federal law or licensing board regulations? Does my conduct breach an ethical principle? Is there a court ruling that imposes a duty on me that is relevant to my conduct? What is the best way to help this particular client, taking into consideration the context of the professional relationship? What should I do to help? What should I not do to help? What are the ramifications of not doing certain things? And what would an average peer, who uses a similar theoretical orientation, working with a similar type of client, with a similar diagnosis or problem, in a comparable type of community, say about my conduct? When appropriate, the records should reflect therapist’s responses or contemplation of these questions. Many of these questions will also get asked in regular supervision, which in some settings is seen as a useful and necessary adjunct to the therapy.

Ethical decision-making in psychotherapy has received much attention because a thorough decision-making process is the important phase in the development of a treatment plan and essential for demonstrating compliance with standard of care. Many texts have focused on the principles of ethics in psychology (e.g., Beauchamp & Childress, 2001). As with the general principles of the American Psychological Association (2016) Ethics Code, many ethicists view the following five moral principles as the foundation of ethical decision making: autonomy, nonmaleficence (i.e., do no harm), beneficence (commitment to benefit the client), justice and fidelity. Several texts outline ethical decision-making for psychotherapists as being broad and inclusive (e.g., Canter, Bennett, Jones, & Nagy, 1996; Corey, Corey, & Callahan, 2003; Herlihy & Corey, 2006; Knapp & VandeCreek, 2006). Other texts focused on ethical decision-making and guidelines with regard to boundary crossings (i.e., Corey, et al., 2003; Gutheil & Gabbard, 1993; Herlihy & Corey, 2006; Knapp & VandeCreek, 2006; Koocher & Keith-Spiegel, 1998; Reamer, 2001; Welfel, 2002). Then some others have provided more specific guidelines, such as those for handling nonsexual touch (Durana, 1998; Hunter & Struve, 1998; McNeil-Haber, 2004; Nordmarken & Zur, 2004; Smith, et al., 1998; Zur, 2007) and some explore the whole issue of ethical touch in psychotherapy (Young, 2005; Zur, 2007).

Demonstrating compliance with the standard of care around touch issues is essentially no different than any other intervention. What may be more relevant to the employment of touch is the issue of consent. Consent to minor and intermittent touch, such as pat on the back or a hug at the end of the session, can be implied or achieved verbally or non-verbally if client initiates it or seems to respond positively. Any consistent touch beyond a handshake, such as greeting and departing hugs or hand holding that is repeated in each or most sessions may be documented in the record with a brief note of explanation for its clinical rationale. Systematic employment of body psychotherapy methods requires both signed written consent by client and clear documentation of the methods employed in the clinical records.

Top of Page
 

Summary

The standard of care is a legal term and has been defined as the customary professional practice in the community. It describes the qualities and conditions that prevail, or should prevail, in a particular mental health service, that a reasonable or average practitioner follows. Most commonly, the standard is defined in legal terms as “that degree of care which a reasonably prudent person would exercise in the same or similar circumstances” (Black, 1990, p. 1405, in Baerger, 2001). It is very important to understand that the standard of care is a minimum and reasonable standard, not a standard of perfection. The standard of care is neither determined by the outcome of therapy, nor is it based on analytic or risk management principles. It calls on practitioners to act in a reasonable, average or “good enough” manner rather than in ideal or perfect ways. The standard of care is derived from the following six elements: State law, licensing board regulations, professional organization codes of ethics, case laws, consensus of the professionals and consensus in the community.

Touch in psychotherapy is a boundary issue. While sexual touch with current clients is boundary violation and always below the standard of care, non-sexual, clinically appropriate touch is a boundary crossing and has wide utility in the treatment of anxiety, depression, trauma and many other mental ailments. Clinically appropriate and ethical touch clearly falls within the standard of care. Clinicians who employ touch in therapy must make sure it is clinically appropriate given the client’s history, age, gender, sexual orientation, culture and presenting problem. They also must take into consideration the type of setting, the quality of the therapeutic relationship, their own comfort and attitudes towards touch and, of course, their training and scope of practice. Consulting with experts can be very beneficial and obtaining some form of consent from their clients is very important. When using techniques that involve touch, it is essential to ensure that appropriate training and supervision has been received. Extensive and systematic use of touch may require signed written informed consent and a rationale given in the clinical records.

In summary, when touch in psychotherapy is employed in clinically and ethically appropriate ways, it clearly falls within the standard of care and has high clinical utility for healing a wide range of ailments and mental disorders.

Top of Page
 

References

American Association for Marriage and Family Therapists. (2015). AAMFT code of ethics. DC, Alexandria, VA: Author. Retrieved from https://www.aamft.org/Legal_Ethics/Code_of_Ethics.aspx

American Counseling Association. (2014). Code of ethics and standards of practice. Alexandria, VA: Author. http://www.counseling.org/Resources/aca-code-of-ethics.pdf.

American Psychological Association (APA). (2016). Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct.

Aponte, J. F., & Wohl, J. (2000). Psychological intervention and cultural diversity. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Aposhyan, S. (2004). Body-Mind Psychotherapy: Principles, Techniques, and Practical Applications. New York: W. W. Norton.

Barshop, C. C. (2005). Body Psychotherapy Modalities: Journal Articles and Online Sources. United States Association of Body Psychotherapy Journal 4/2, 116-145.

Beauchamp, T. L., & Childress, J. F. (2001). Principles of biomedical ethics (5th ed.). New York: Oxford University Press

Bersoff, D. N. (1994). Explicit ambiguity: The 1992 ethics code as oxymoron. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 25, 382-387.

Bersoff, D. (1999). Ethical conflicts in psychology. (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Black, H. C. (1990). Black’s Law Dictionary. (6th ed.). St. Paul, MN: West Publishing Co.

Borys, D. S., & Pope, K. S. (1989). Dual relationships between therapist and client: A national study of psychologists, psychiatrists, and social workers. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 20, 283-293.

Bowlby, J. (1952). Maternal care and mental health: A report on behalf of the World Health Organization. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization.

Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment, separation and loss (Vol. 1). New York: Basic Books.

Brodsky, A. M. (1985). Sex between therapists and patients: Ethical gray areas. Psychotherapy in Private Practice, 3(1), 57-62.

Caldwell, C. (1997). Getting in touch: The guide to new body-centered therapies. Wheaton, IL: Quest Books.

Canter, M., Bennett, B., Jones, S., & Nagy, T. (1996). Ethics for psychologists. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Caudill, C. O. (2004). Therapists under fire. Retrieved June 1, 2004, from http://www.cphins.com/Default.aspx?tabid=75

Corey, G., Corey, M. S., & Callahan, P. (2003). Issues and ethics in the helping professions (6th ed.). Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.

Cornell, W. F. (1997). Touch and boundaries in transactional analysis: Ethical and transferential considerations. Transactional Analysis Journal, 37(1), 30–37.

Craig, J. D. (1991). Preventing dual relationships in pastoral counseling. Counseling and Values, 36, 49-55.

Dineen, T. (1996). Manufacturing victims: What the psychology industry is doing to people. Westmount, Quebec, Canada: Robert Davies.

Downey, D. L. (2001). Therapeutic touch in psychotherapy. Psychotherapy, 36/1, 35-38.

Dunne, C., Bruggen, P., & O’Brien, C. (1982). Touch and action in group therapy of younger adolescents. Journal of Adolescents, 5, 31–38.

Durana, C. (1998). The use of touch in psychotherapy: Ethical and clinical guidelines. Psychotherapy, 35/2, 269-280.

European Association Body-Psychotherapists (2006). Body Psychotherapy. Retrieved on Feb. 1, 2007, from http://www.eabp.org

Fay, A. (2002). The case against boundaries in psychotherapy. In A. A. Lazarus & O. Zur (Eds.), Dual relationships and psychotherapy (pp. 146–166). New York: Springer Publishing Company.

Field, T. (1998) Massage therapy effects. American Psychologist, 53, 12, 1270-1281.

Field, T. (2003). Touch. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Fosshage, J. L., (2000). The meanings of touch in psychoanalysis: A time for reassessment. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 20/1. Retrieved July 1, 2004, from http://www.psychoanalyticinquiry.com/vol20no1.html

Gabbard, G. O. (1994). Teetering on the precipice: A commentary on Lazarus’s “How certain boundaries and ethics diminish therapeutic effectiveness.” Ethics & Behavior, 4(3), 283-286.

Gabbard, G. O. (1996). Lessons to be learned from the study of sexual boundary violations. American Journal of Psychotherapy, 50, 311-322.

Gutheil, T. G. (1989, November/December). Patient–therapist sexual relations. The California Therapist, 29–39.

Gutheil, T. G. (1998). The psychiatrist as expert witness. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.

Gutheil, T. G., & Gabbard, G. O. (1993). The concept of boundaries in clinical practice: Theoretical and risk-management dimensions. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 188-196.

Halbrook, B., & Duplechin, R. (1994). Rethinking touch in psychotherapy: Guidelines for practitioners. Osychotherapy in Private Practice, 13/3, 43-53.

Harlow, H. (1971). Learning to love. New York: Albion.

Hedges, L. E. (2000). Facing The Challenge of Liability in Psychotherapy. New Jersey: Janson Aronson, Inc.

Hedges, L. E., Hilton, R., Hilton, V. W., & Caudill, O. B. (1997). Therapists at risk: Perils of the intimacy of the therapeutic relationships. Northvale, NJ: Jason Aronson.

Heller, S. (1997). The Vital Touch: How Intimate Contact With Your Baby Leads To Happier, Healthier Development. New York:

Herlihy, B., & Corey, G. (2006). Boundary issues in counseling: Multiple roles and responsibilities (2nd ed.). Alexandria, VA: American Association for Counseling and Development.

Hollinger, L. (1986). Communicating with the elderly. Journal of Gerontological Nursing, 12(3), 8–13.

Holroyd, J. C., & Brodsky, A. (1977). Psychologists’ attitudes and practices regarding erotic and nonerotic physical contact with patients. American Psychologist, 32, 843-849

Holroyd, J. C., & Brodsky, A. (1980). Does touching patients lead to sexual intercourse? Professional Psychology, 11, 807–811.

Holub E., Lee S. (1990). Therapists’ use of nonerotic physical contact: Ethical Concerns. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 21/2, 115-117.

Horton, J., Clance P. R., Sterk-Elifson, C., Emshoff, J., (1995). Touch in psychotherapy: A survey of patients’ experiences. Psychotherapy, 32, 443-457.

Hunter, M., & Struve, J. (1998). The ethical use of touch in psychotherapy. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Knapp, S. J., & VandeCreek, L. D. (2006). Practical ethics for psychologists: A positive approach. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Koocher, G. P. (2006). Forward to the second edition: Things my teachers never mentioned. In K. S. Pope, J. L. Sonne, & B. Greene. What therapists don’t talk about and why: Understanding taboos that hurt us and our clients (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Koocher, G. P., & Keith-Spiegel, P. (1998). Ethics in psychology: Professional standards and cases (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.

Lakin, M. (1991). Coping with ethical dilemmas in psychotherapy. New York: Pergamon Press.

Lambert, M. J. (1991). Introduction to psychotherapy research. In L. E. Beutler & M. Cargo (Eds.), Psychotherapy research: An international review of programmatic studies (pp. 1–11). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Langs, R. (1982). Psychotherapy: A basic text. New York: Aronson.

LaPierre, A. (2003). From felt-sense to felt-self: Neuroaffective touch and the relational matrix. Psychologist-Psychoanalyst, Official Publication of Division 39 (Psychoanalysis) of the American Psychological Association, 13/4.

LaPierre, A. (2006). Neuroscience book review: Part III neuroscience in somatic psychotherapy. United States Association of Body Psychotherapy Journal, 5, 43-60.

LaTorre, M. (2005). The use of Reiki in psychotherapy. Perspectives in Psychiatric Care, 41(4), 184-187.

Lawry, S., (1998). Touch and clients who have been sexually abused. In Hunter & Struve (Eds.), The Ethical Use of Touch in Psychotherapy. New York: Guilford Press.

Lazarus, A. A. (1994). How certain boundaries and ethics diminish therapeutic effectiveness. Ethics and Behavior, 4, 255-261.

Lazarus, A. A., & Zur, O. (Eds.). (2002). Dual relationships and psychotherapy. New York: Springer Publishing Company.

Levine, P., & Frederick, A. (1997). Waking the tiger: Healing trauma: The innate capacity to transform overwhelming experiences. Berkeley, CA: North Atlantic Books.

Levitan, A., & Johnson, J. (1986). The role of touch in healing and hypnotherapy. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 28 (4), 218-223.

Lowen, A. (1958). Physical dynamics of character structure. New York: Greenwood.

Lowen, A. (1976). Bioenergetics. New York: Penguin.

Marten, J. (2006). Physical holding in psychotherapy. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Family Therapy, 27(2) 119-127.

May, J. (2005). The outcome of body psychotherapy. United States Association of Body Psychotherapy Journal 4/2.

McNeil-Haber, F. M. (2004). Ethical considerations in the use of nonerotic touch in psychotherapy with children. Ethics & Behavior. 14(2), 123-140.

Menninger, K. (1958). Theory of psychoanalytic technique. New York: Basic Books.

Milakovitch, J. C. (1993). Touching in psychotherapy: The differences between therapists who touch and those who do not. Dissertation Abstracts International, 54(6-B), 3347.

Montagu, A. (1986). Touching: The human significance of the skin. (3rd ed.). New York: Harper & Row.

Nordmarken, N., & Zur, O. (2004). To touch or not to touch: Rethinking the prohibition on touch in psychotherapy and counseling: Clinical, ethical and legal considerations. Retrieved August 5, 2005, from https://www.zurinstitute.com/touchintherapy.html

Perls, F. (1973). The Gestalt approach and eye witness to therapy. Palo Alto, CA: Science & Behavior Books.

Phillips, J. (2002). Somatic tracking and the ethical use of touch. United States Association of Body Psychotherapy Journal 1, 63-77.

Pope, K. S. (1990). Therapist-patient sex as sex abuse: Six scientific, professional, and practical dilemmas in addressing victimization and rehabilitation. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 21, 227-239.

Pope, K. S., & Vasquez, M. J. T. (2007). Ethics in psychotherapy and counseling: A practical guide (3rd ed.). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Pope, K. S., Tabachnick, B. G., & Keith-Spiegel, K. (1987). Ethics of practice: The beliefs and behaviors of psychologists as therapists. American Psychologist, 42, 993–1006.

Pope, K. S., Sonne, J. L., & Holroyd, J. (1993). Sexual feelings in psychotherapy: Explorations for therapists and therapists-in-training. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Reamer, F. G. (2001). Tangled relationships: Managing boundary issues in human services. New York: Columbia University Press.

Reich, W. (1972). Character analysis. New York: Simon & Schuster.

Reid, W. H. (1998). Standard of care and patient need. The Journal of Psychiatric Practice, May. Retrieved June 4, 2004, from www.reidpsychiatry.com/columns/Reid05-98.pdf

Rogers, C. (1970). Carl Rogers on encounter groups. New York: Harper & Row.

Rutter, P. (1989). Sex in the forbidden zone: When men in power—therapists, doctors, clergy, teachers, and others–betray women’s trust. New York: Psychiatric Annals, 21, 614-619. New York: Fawcett Crest.

Satir, V. (1972). Peoplemaking. Palo Alto, CA: Science & Behavior Books.

Schank, A. J., & Skovholt, T. M. (2006). Ethical practice in small communities: Challenges and rewards for psychologist. Washington DC: American Psychological Association.

Schore, A. (2003). Affect regulation and the repair of the self. New York: W.W. Norton.

Schultz, L. G. (1975). A survey of social workers’ attitudes and use of body and sex psychotherapies. Clinical Social Work Journal, 3, 90–99.

Simon, R. I. (1991). Psychological injury caused by boundary violation precursors to therapist-patient sex. Psychiatric Annals, 21, 614-619.

Simon, R. I. (1994). Transference in therapist–patient sex: The illusion of patient improvement and consent, Part 1. Psychiatric Annals, 24, 509–515.

Simon, R. I. (2001). Psychiatry and the law for clinicians. (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.

Smith, E., Clance, P. R. & Imes, S. (Eds.) (1998) Touch in psychotherapy: Theory, research and practice. New York: Guilford Press.

Sonne, J. L. (1994). Multiple relationships: Does the new ethics code answer the right questions? Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 25, 331–343.

St. Germaine, J. (1996). Dual relationships and certified alcohol and drug counselors: A national study of ethical beliefs and behaviors. Alcoholism Treatment Quarterly, 14(2), 29-45.

Stake, J. E., & Oliver, J. (1991). Sexual contact and touching between therapist and client: A survey of psychologists’ attitudes and behavior. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 22, 297-307.

Stenzel, C. L., & Rupert, P. A. (2004). Psychologists’ use of touch in individual psychotherapy. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice and Training, 41, 332-345.

Strasburger, L. H., Jorgenson, L., & Sutherland, P. (1992). The prevention of psychotherapist sexual misconduct: Avoiding the slippery slope. American Journal of Psychotherapy, 46(4), 544-555.

Sue, D., & Sue, D. (2003). Counseling the culturally diverse: Theory and practice. (4th ed.). New York: John Wiley and Sons.

Tirnauer, L., Smith, E., & Foster, P. (1996). The American Academy of Psychotherapists research committee survey of members. Voices, 32(2), 87-94.

Toronto, E. L. K. (2001). The human touch: An exploration of the role and meaning of physical touch in psychoanalysis. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 18, 37-54.

Totton, N. (ed) (2005) New dimensions in Body Psychotherapy. Maidenhead, UK, Open University Press

United States Association for Body Psychotherapy. (2001). Ethics guidelines. Bethesda, MD: Author. Retrieved July 1, 2005, from http://usabp.org/about-us/usabp-code-of-ethics/

United States Association of Body Psychotherapy. (2006). Definition of Body Psychotherapy. Retrieved on Feb. 1, 2007, from http://www.usabp.org/displaycommon.cfm?an=1&subarticlenbr=9

WebMD, (1992). Way too personal: The temptation and consequences of patient-therapist sex. By Colleen Bates Dunn, WebMD feature. Retrieved July 1, 2004, from http://aolsvc.health.webmd.aol.com/content/Article/14/1687_50119.htm?printing=true

Welfel, E. R. (2002). Ethics in counseling and psychotherapy: Standards, research, and emerging issues (2nd ed.). Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.

White, K. E. (2002). A study of ethical and clinical implications for the appropriate use of touch in psychotherapy. United States Association of Body Psychotherapy Journal, 1, 16-41.

Williams, M. H. (1997). Boundary violations: Do some contended standards of care fail to encompass commonplace procedures of Humanistic, Behavioral, and Eclectic Psychotherapies? Psychotherapy, 34(3), 238-249.

Williams, M. H. (2000). Victimized by “Victims:” A Taxonomy of Antecedents of False Complaints Against Psychotherapists. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 31 (1), 75-8

Williams, M. H., (2003). The curse of risk management. The Independent Practitioner, 23(4), 202-205.

Wolberg, L. (1967). The technique of psychotherapy (2nd ed.). New York: Grune & Stratton.

Woody, R. H. (1998). Bartering for psychological services. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 29, 174–178.

Wrye, H., & Welles, J. (1994). The narration of desire: Erotic transferences and countertransferences. Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press.

Young, C. (1997). Body Psychotherapy: It’s history and present day scope. An address to the European Association for Body Psychotherapy Congress, Rome, Common Ground and Different Approaches in Psychotherapy. Retrieved on Feb. 1, 2007, from http://www.usabp.org/displaycommon.cfm?an=1&subarticlenbr=11

Young, C. (2005). About the ethics of professional touch. Retrieved on January 3, 2007, from http://www.eabp.org/pdf/TheEthicsofTouch.pdf.

Younggren, J. N., & Gottlieb, M. C. (2004). Managing risk when contemplating multiple relationships. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 35, 255–260.

Zur, O. (2000). In celebration of dual relationships: How prohibition of non-sexual dual relationships increases the chance of exploitation and harm. The Independent Practitioner, 2(3), 97–100.

Zur, O. (2001). Out-of-office experience: When crossing office boundaries and engaging in dual relationships are clinically beneficial and ethically sound. Independent Practitioner, 21(1), 96-100.

Zur, O. (2005). The HIPAA Compliance Kit: Understanding and applying the regulations in psychotherapeutic practice. (3rd ed.). New York: W. W. Norton.

Zur, O. (2007). Boundaries in psychotherapy: Ethical and clinical explorations. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

 
Top of Page

Sign up for topical updates and invitations to participate with Dr. Zur